Peter Garrard, Associate Professor in Neurology, St George's, University of London, talks to Cardio Debate about young onset dementia. Transcript Definition and how to diagnose – any diagnostic markers available in everyday practice? It’s somewhat arbitrary but we think of young onset dementia as dementia that occurs within working age, so under the age of 65 years. It’s a somewhat arbitrary definition and actually the statistics show demographically is that the instance and prevalence on dementia is very low in this working-age age group – so less than one per cent of the population. And then as people age the incidence increases by five per cent per year, so that over the age of 90 it’s estimated that around a quarter or even one third of people meet the criteria for dementia. But the significance of this is that it is a disease of aging, for which ageing is one of the major risk factors. So when we see a patient in general practice with symptoms that are suggestive of cognitive failure – so memory impairment, language loss, behavior change, inability to navigate or use visual apparatus to perform the tasks of daily life – its always regarded as a low probability that they’re going to have anything seriously wrong with them. Whereas in an older person, because the prior probability is so high, a diagnosis of dementia will be suspected much more often – so there’s often a delay. I think the most important biomarkers to look out for at a clinical level, the characteristic symptom complex, characteristic symptom areas – is to realize that dementia doesn’t always present with memory loss or forgetfulness. It can present with difficulty in navigation, difficulty with using visual information – difficulty with reading, with driving, with navigating around the house. And this is quite frequently is dismissed as being a symptom of visual impairment, which it isn’t – it’s visual processing impairment. That must always be taken seriously. And then there are language symptoms – changes in word finding ability, changes in ability to understand more complex words. And there are these rather unusual behavioural symptoms, where people’s personalities change from being compatible with leading a normal life in social work environments, with beginning to exhibit bizarre behaviours. Mechanisms of young onset dementia – is there a genetic background involved? With regard to the underlying mechanism of these conditions, there are – we think of them in terms of underlying pathology – and overall the commonest the underlying pathology causing dementia is going to be Alzheimer’s disease. But overall, and in a younger onset population, those patients with the underlying pathology of Alzheimer’s disease would account for maybe one third of the population, and of the remainder a large percentage will probably be caused by premature vascular disease, and then perhaps an equally large percentage with non-Alzheimer neurodegenerative conditions. There are also groups of patients with non-neurodegenerative conditions, inflammatory conditions, toxic conditions, alcoholism, all presenting in that age group with cognitive decline. But the non-Alzheimer neurodegenerative conditions fall into two distinct subtypes, and they are defined according to the protein that accumulates in the brain. So with Alzheimer’s disease as everyone knows it’s Amyloid-Tau clots and tangles, and in the non-Alzheimer neurodegenerative cases there’s one group where Tau alone without Amyloid accumulates, and we call them tauopathies. And there’s another group where the accumulating abnormal protein is something known as TDP43. And Tau and TDP43 both accumulate principally in frontal and temporal regions, so they’re front-of-brain neurodegenerative syndromes, whereas Alzheimer’s is more posterior. So with Alzheimer’s we see more visual and memory problems, but with the anterior ones wee see more language and behavior problems. Now, the mechanisms in the vast majority of all these underlying pathologies are unknown. But a proportion of them will be accounted for by having inherited a faulty gene, which are normally autosomally dominantly transmitted. So those ones will be easily recognized as they will give a very reliable and good family history, both of their own generation and their parents generation, possibly further back too. But those only account for about 5 to 10 per cent, probably, of all the young onset dementias. Perhaps slightly more in the non-Alzheimer group. But by far the majority are sporadic, and the causes of sporadic neurodegenerative syndromes are totally unknown, and they are a source of intensive research across the world. And the first person who finds out a mechanism for sporadic Alzheimer’s or tauopathy or TDP43 will become very famous indeed, very quickly. To read the full transcript visit www.cardio-debate.com
In this talk Peter describes his journey from classics to computational linguistics, by way of neuroscience. He explains how simple transformations of texts by a computer can expose unseen information reflecting a writer’s cognitive status, mental life, and even personality, at the time of composition. He refers to this approach as ‘cognitive archaeology’, and will illustrate its potential importance using examples of his own analyses and those of others whose work he has encountered along the way. He will outline past and future contributions of cognitive archaeology to literary scholarship, psychology, history, neurology and even politics. Dr Peter Garrard is a neurologist and neuroscientist at St George's Hospital Medical School. His research and clinical practice at focus on dementia and its effects on speech and language. Notable research subjects include the novelist Iris Murdoch, former Prime Minister Harold Wilson, and King George III. More recent research has used computer algorithms to demonstrate language changes associated with political power: changes were seen in the speeches of two British politicians who developed the 'Hubris syndrome'. This recently-described personality disorder leads to dangerous overconfidence, isolation and misjudgement in certain leaders. Dr Garrard will show how the language used by two recent British Prime Ministers reflects the onset and time course of the Hubris syndrome. He will suggest ways in which language analysis may help in monitoring personality traits of political, business and financial leaders. This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx
You can enable or disable according to the purposes: